For thousands of years, games have been central to human life. From cards to puzzles to board games, many people use them as a way to socialize and exercise their brains.
It turns out bumblebees might be doing the same.
A new study from researchers at the Queen Mary University of London shows that members of a bumblebee species were able to learn from one another how to finish a puzzle — a sign that the buzzing critters could have a “capacity for culture.” While the phenomenon has been studied in animals like birds and monkeys, the findings — published Tuesday in PLOS Biology — provide a look at the potential for culture in invertebrate creatures like bumblebees.
“Even though they have these brains that are literally the size of a pinhead — they’re absolutely tiny — they still have some of the most intricate and complicated behavioral repertoires on the entire planet,” said Alice Bridges, the study’s lead author and a lecturer of biology and animal behavior at Anglia Ruskin University in England.
Advertisement
Bridges has been studying bumblebees since 2017, when she started her doctoral degree at Queen Mary University of London. Like most people, she’d always thought the insects were “really cute” but became more fascinated as she learned about their behavior and processes.
The first step for the study, which began in 2018, was to design a puzzle for the bees.
For their experiments, Bridges and the research team created a round puzzle out of Petri dishes that could be opened two different ways. If the bees opened the container, they could get the reward inside it — sugar water.
To see if the Bombus terrestris could learn from one another, the Queen Mary University team trained a few bees to solve the puzzle, using only one of the two options. She made sure the bees that were selected knew how to push the tab in the puzzle to get to the sweet reward.
Advertisement
Bridges then sent the trained bees back to their respective colonies. A little while after that, each colony was brought to the puzzle.
As the bees in each colony solved the puzzle, the team realized the trained bees had shared what they learned.
“Some of them would play around with the box and find the alternative solution, but they still would revert really strongly back to the demonstrator’s preferred behavior,” Bridges said. “So, this essentially meant we were seeing the establishment of like a local trend.”
The way humans define it, “culture” is quite complex, she added. But she said the “common denominator” of both human and nonhuman culture is behavior that is learned from someone else, or social learning.
“It’s not innate or instinctive,” Bridges said. “You have to learn it.”
During the scientific process, Bridges got a couple of bee stings that led to hives. After the fourth sting, she had a reaction so bad, she had to go to the hospital.
Advertisement
After developing the allergy, she started wearing a bee suit to work and getting weekly shots. And she hired research assistants to work directly with the puzzle containers so she wouldn’t have direct contact with the bumblebees.
While doing those things got “pretty intense” at times, Bridges said it was worth it for the team’s breakthrough.
Bumblebee populations in recent years have been dropping because of rising temperatures. With that threat in mind, Bridges hopes the team’s study can be used for conservation efforts — perhaps as insight into how some bees can learn new behaviors in their rapidly changing environment.
“It is something that can have really big implications,” she said. “Not just for conservation, but also, I think, our understanding of ourselves and our place in all of this.”
This website or its third-party tools use cookies, which are necessary for analytics and its functioning and required to achieve the purposes illustrated in the cookie policy, including the personalization and analysis of ads and content. If you want to learn more or withdraw your consent to all or some of the cookies, please refer to the cookie policy.
By clicking on Accept you are agreeing to the placement of cookies on your device.
Further use of our site shall be considered as consent. You may view our Privacy Policy and Cookie Policy here for more information.
This website uses cookies so that we can provide you with the best user experience possible. Cookie information is stored in your browser and performs functions such as recognising you when you return to our website and helping our team to understand which sections of the website you find most interesting and useful.
Strictly Necessary Cookies
Strictly Necessary Cookie should be enabled at all times so that we can save your preferences for cookie settings.
If you disable this cookie, we will not be able to save your preferences. This means that every time you visit this website you will need to enable or disable cookies again.